On January 17, 2019 The New York
Times, in the person of one of the newspaper’s premiere columnists, David
Brooks, discovered social and emotional learning. In a column titled “Students
Learn from People They Love,” Brooks summarizes some of the research that over
the last few decades has gotten us to appreciate the role of emotion in
learning and thus the importance of the quality of the relationship between
teachers and their students. “We used to have this top-down notion that reason
was on a teeter-totter with emotion,” writes Brooks. “If you wanted to be
rational and think well, you had to suppress those primitive gremlins, the
emotions.”
Brooks goes on to describe various
studies in neuroscience that demonstrate the beneficial interrelationship
between learning and emotion. He then asks “How many recent ed reform trends
have been about relationship building?... We focus on all the wrong things
because we have an outmoded conception of how thinking really works.”
I monitored my own emotions as I
read Mr. Brooks’ column. I was pleased to see someone with a politically
conservative bent celebrating the importance of the teacher-student
relationship, for in the past, many conservative commentators have decried what
they've seen as the abandonment of academic content in education in favor of “softer,”
more social and developmental goals and outcomes. And it was good to see
someone with a national platform challenging the commonplace reductive
dichotomy between reason and emotion.
But I also found myself thinking
what I often think when reading about breakthrough educational research in
neuroscience and other fields. In this case, do we need all these studies to
demonstrate what any good teacher knows: that the nature and quality of the
relationship between teachers and students matters? Thus do the wheels of
education policy turn in our country.
More broadly I worry that as we pay
needed attention to the full scope of a child’s being, we will inadvertently
reinforce the false dichotomy between thought and emotion that Mr. Brooks
decries. Mr. Brooks concludes his column with “[t]he good news” that “the
social and emotional learning movement has been steadily gaining strength.”
He’s right. We have books, conferences, consultants, and a proliferation of
Internet platforms dedicated to social and emotional learning, some of which,
unfortunately, trade in simplified notions of the way the mind works.
As if to illustrate how easily we
slip into reductive binaries about mental activity—this is a cultural danger in
the West—Mr. Brooks concludes his column with praise for a practice mentioned
in a new report from the Aspen Institute: “Some schools… do no academic
instruction the first week. To start, everybody just gets to know one another.”
Why, I wonder, would we need to suspend instruction to get to know one
another—unless anything academic is seen as antithetical to human relation and
instruction is conceived in a narrow way? How did we arrive at the place where
writing or doing science or talking about a painting or a song is viewed as
incompatible with “getting to know one another?”
A few years back, I posted a blog
exploring the shriveled notion of cognition that has resulted from the last two
decades of education policy. I repost it here—with apologies for my tendency to
repost. It’s just that some issues seem to appear and reappear with the
regularity of the seasons. The piece below, “Giving Cognition a Bad Name”
(originally posted February 19, 2013) was written during a period of national
enthusiasm for character education. As you read it, you can substitute the
construct “social and emotional learning” for “character education” and get a
sense of what concerns me today about some of the discussions of social and
emotional learning. Please understand, I am not at all disputing the importance
of the social and emotional dimensions of teaching and learning; much of what
I’ve written over the years has been an attempt to articulate and give texture
to these aspects of classroom life. Rather, I’m trying to underscore the
intricate interweave of thought and feeling when we teach and when we learn.
***
Giving Cognition a Bad Name
Cognition traditionally refers to a
wide and rich range of mental processes, from memory and attention, to
comprehending and using language, to solving a difficult problem in physics or
choreography or living with someone. But over the last few decades cognition
has been reduced to a shadow of its former self. Under No Child Left Behind and
Race to the Top, cognition in education policy has increasingly come to mean
the skills measured by standardized tests of reading and mathematics. And as
economists have gotten more involved in education, they’ve needed quantative
measures of cognitive ability and academic achievement for their analytical
models, so they’ve used I.Q. or other standardized test scores (like the Armed
Forces Qualification Test or AFQT) as a proxy for intelligence or achievement.
From the Latin cognoscere, to come to
know, or cogito erqo sum, I think
therefore I am, we’ve devolved to a few digits on the AFQT.
As if that were not enough, there is
now emerging on a number of fronts – nicely summarized in Paul Tough’s new book
How Children Succeed – a belief that our nation’s educational focus on
cognition has been misguided. Rather than focusing our energies on the academic
curriculum – or on academic intervention programs for the poor – we need to
turn our attention to the development of qualities of character or personality
like perseverance, self-monitoring, and flexibility. As much or more than the
cognitive, the argument goes, it is these qualities that account for success in
school and life.
It is healthy to be reminded about
the fuller scope of education in our test- and grade-obsessed culture, and I
must admit a guilty pleasure in watching someone as smart as Nobel Laureate in
economics, James Heckman (one of the advocates for character education) go
after our current Department of Education’s reductive academic policies.
The importance of qualities like
perseverance and flexibility are indisputable, but what concerns me is that the
advocates for character accept without question the reductive notion of
cognition that runs through our education policies, and by accepting it further
affirm it. The problem is exacerbated by the aforementioned way economists
carve up and define mental activity. If cognition is represented by scores on
ability or achievement tests, then anything not captured in those scores – like
the desired qualities of character – is, de facto, non-cognitive. We’re now left with a pinched notion of cognition
and a reductive dichotomy to boot.
This downplaying of the cognitive and the
construction of the cognitive/non-cognitive binary will have some troubling
implications for education, especially the education of the children of the
poor.
To begin with, the labeling of character
qualities as “non-cognitive” misrepresents them – particularly if you use the
truer, richer notion of cognition. Self-monitoring, for example, has to involve
a consideration and analysis of one’s performance and mental state – a
profoundly cognitive activity. Flexibility demands a weighing of options and
decision-making. This is not just a problem of terminology, for if you don’t have
an accurate description of something, how can you help people develop it,
especially if you want to scale up your efforts?
Furthermore, these desired qualities are
developed over time in settings and relationships that are meaningful to the
participants, which most likely means that the settings and relationships will
have significant cognitive content. Two of the classic pre-school programs that
have provided a research base for the character advocates – the Perry Preschool
and Abecedarian Projects – were cognitively rich in imaginative play, language
use, and activities that required thought and cooperation. A very different
example comes from a study I just completed observing community college
occupational programs as varied as fashion and diesel technology. As students
developed competence, they also became more committed to doing a job well, were
better able to monitor and correct their performance, and improved their
ability to communicate what they were doing and help others do it. You could be
by inclination the most dogged or communicative person in the world, but if you
don’t know what you’re doing with a garment or an engine, you’re tendencies
won’t be realized in a meaningful way in the classroom or the workshop.
Also, we have to consider the consequences of
this cognitive/ non-cognitive binary in light of the history of American
educational practice. We have a powerful tendency toward either/or policies –
think of old math/new math or phonics/whole language. Given this tendency, we
can predict a pendulum swing away from the academic and toward character
education. And over the past fifty years attempts at character education as a
distinct pursuit have not been particularly successful.
Finally, the focus of the current character
education movement is on low-income children, and the cold, hard fact is that
many poor kids are already getting terrible educations in the cognitive domain.
There’s a stirring moment in Paul Tough’s book where a remarkable chess teacher
decides she’s going to try to prepare one of her star pupils for an admissions
test for New York’s selective high schools. What she found was that this
stunningly bright boy had learned pitifully little academic knowledge during
his eight years in school. It would be tragic to downplay a strong academic
education for children like him.
By all means, let us take a hard look at
our national obsession with tests and scores and grades, and let us think more
generously about what kinds of people we want our schools to develop. Part of
such reconsideration would include a reclaiming of the full meaning of
cognition, a meaning that is robust and vitally intellectual, intimately
connected to character and social development, and directed toward the creation
of a better world.
You can share this blog post on Facebook, Twitter, or Google Reader through the "share" function located at the top left-hand corner of the blog.